| 
  • If you are citizen of an European Union member nation, you may not use this service unless you are at least 16 years old.

  • You already know Dokkio is an AI-powered assistant to organize & manage your digital files & messages. Very soon, Dokkio will support Outlook as well as One Drive. Check it out today!

View
 

Chapter 6

Page history last edited by Kimberley Kennedy 13 years, 9 months ago

Established & edited by Paula Benter & Kimberley Castaneda 

Extended by Shawnyel Haywood & Audrey Ochoa

 

 

Chapter 6: Impact of Preschool and Kindergarten

 

Programs on

 

Young Children's Early Literacy Skills

 

     

 

Purpose of this Chapter:

 

The panel wanted to determine if early childhood programs and preschools offered an advantage in literacy learning or developed early skills that could be used to later predict literacy success. Can we determine what students need to become on-level readers based on their individual characteristics: socioeconomic status, ethnicity, home language, or community?

 

 

Programs and Studies Involved in the Meta-Analysis: 

 

Several early childhood programs are named specifically in this chapter of the report including:

  • Head Start 
  • The Abecedarian project
  • The Chicago Child-Parent Center
  • The HighScope Preschool Project  

From these and various other school based programs, studies were collected that included programs or interventions that looked at the development of early literacy and conventional literacy skills. The panel was very careful about the studies that were allowed into the analysis, so when looking at outcomes it is important to understand that in some categories very few studies met the requirements of the panel.  It is also important to mention that none of the studies that were acceptable to the panel included Head Start.

  

 

Literacy Categories (Dependent Variables) and Number of Studies Used

Alphabet Knowledge

4

Cognitive Ability

4

Memory

2

Oral Language

12

Phonological Awareness

2

Print Knowledge

2

Readiness

3

Reading

9

Spelling

3

Writing

2

 

 

Preschool and Kindergarten Study Summary:

 

Due to the limited number of studies that were looked at and the omission of large national preschool programs like Head Start, this section of the study on predicting later literacy outcomes did not have statistical significance.  However, there are several fabulous messages here about preschool programs that are very important.

 

The greatest impact was on the composite measure for reading readiness which showed that preschool and kindergarten programs are highly effective in getting children ready for school. A smaller but still important effect was the ability of kindergarten programs to assist in the development of spelling skills. This may be due to the focus of kindergarten on spelling, but may also be influenced by preschools development of phonemic awareness skills and alphabetic knowledge.

 

It is interesting to note that although several areas did not reach statistical significance the researchers mention that the areas of the study are still educationally significant. This happens because of the number of studies used and the way the statistics are calculated. For instance, the oral language outcomes are very modest, but this could be because that was not the main outcome focus for the study included. Oral language is still an important component of early childhood education; however, the study could only say statistically that these programs can have an impact on the reading development of children. 

 

The panel wanted to determine if age, ethnicity, or population density affected the outcomes for later literacy success and calculated statistics for each of these areas; however, the studies that were included again got in the way. At first in was believed that kindergartners did better than preschoolers by age, but the statistics overlapped and this could not be proven. Ethnicity was not reported in several studies and the data could not be interpreted. Finally, population density was not specified in several studies and the ones that did specify urban did not have outcomes for the variable that was studied, so the data was not statistically reliable.

 

Lastly, the panel wanted to inform the preschool and kindergarten community about the importance of literacy-focused curricula, professional development, and parental involvement. They found that literacy-focused curricula and professional development were very important for successful programs. In particular a literacy-focused curriculum was pivotal for reading readiness. Unfortunately, the studies included both literacy-focused curricula and professional development together and the variables could not be separated.

Parental involvement is heavily used in several of the program studies used and the panel dedicated a chapter to shared-reading; however, in the context of early childhood preschool settings it could not be reliably measured or empirically proven that parental involvement improved literacy performance. That does not mean that it is not important to encourage parental involvement. It simply means that statistically researchers have not found a good way to measure this variable independently.

 

 

Implications:

 

This study brings to light the need for more experimental and quasiexperimental research. Much of what is studied is qualitative in nature and in changing governmental understanding more numerical data is needed. It would be beneficial if that data can include more specifically age, ethnicity, population density, and socioeconomic status. This could allow for richer data to inform decisions made at the governmental level.

 

 

 

  

Preschool Programs:  

  

The Abecedarian Project                                                         

(Campbell & Ramey 1994)(Campbell & Ramey, 1995)                                        

 

          This project took place in a university town in the United States between 1972 and 1977. To look at an historical perspective of the Abecedarian Project click here. The researchers combined sociodemographic factors and a High Risk Index to determine eligibility of families and children. In addition to these at-risk indicators, children were assessed to be free from any links to mental retardation. Each family chosen was found to be living in poverty with 98% of the participants being African American. 109 families total were enrolled into the program along with their 111 total children with the average age being 4.4 months. 

           The center was open for eight hours per day, five days per week, and approximately 50 weeks per year. Upon entering the study each child was placed into a group. These groups differed in treatment given in relation to academics.

            In the preschool program, a curriculum was used that would, “enhance cognitive, language, perceptual- motor, and social development” (Campbell & Ramey, 1994, p. 687). This same curriculum was adapted as children grew older to include preliteracy skills and language development. The activities were considered learning games and this program was tailored to the students as they were ready for each lesson. Once children reached school age, they were assigned a Home School Resource Teacher. This teacher was the liaison between the family and the school and would make weekly visits to the child’s classroom and discuss the child’s current success with the homeroom teacher. These discussions would then lead to the Home School Resource teacher to make packets for the child and parent to work with at home to enhance the child’s learning.

            Like the others, this study kept in contact with the participants and tested them several times throughout their academic lives. The academic gains found within this study lasted through seven years of schooling and those children (infancy to age 12) maintained a higher IQ over those children who did not participate in the study. Academic test performance was significantly higher for those students who participated in the study in the areas of both reading and math. Also, there were fewer instances of referrals to special education and grade level retention in the participant group than the control group.  

 

  • Children from families of low income were provided high quality education from ages 0-5 years of age. 
  • Educational activities consisted of games to teach cognitive, language, motor and social development,
  • Postive long lasting affects were associated with the program. 

 

  

 

The HighScope Preschool Project

(Schweinhart & Weikart, 1997)

          

             In this 1967 study, Weikart and his colleagues assessed the effects of three diverse preschool curriculum models on young children living in poverty. The study was conducted in Ypsilanti, Michigan and researchers used a three question questionnaire to judge poverty levels and eligibility to the study. Children, ages three and four, were only admitted when they were perceived to score low on the Stanford- Binet Intelligence scale and had no evidence of a physical disability. In total 68 children were selected and were randomly assigned to three different curriculum groups- High/Scope, Direct Instruction, and traditional nursery school.

            The Direct Instruction curriculum model that was used was originally developed by Bereiter and Engelmann in 1966 and represented a type of programmed-learning approach to education. This method of instruction focused on the teaching of the academic skills that were represented on achievement and intelligence tests. This model was implemented by teachers using twenty-minute lessons in the areas of mathematics, reading, and language. In these well planned and precisely prepared lessons, the focus was on questions and answers without excess dialogue.  The classrooms did not contain much else besides the teachers’ manual and student workbooks, as these were the materials used to learn.

            The nursery school curriculum model is a child-centered approach in which the teachers center learning on units of study and themes. The focus in this curriculum model is on developing social skills rather than intellectual skills. This approach is considered child-centered because although the teacher guides each unit of study, the children are able to make choices about some of the activities and paths that they take that individualizes their learning.

            The High/Scope curriculum model is based on the work of Piaget and adults engage children as active learners in the different interest areas situated throughout the classroom. Each day, the children completed a cycle of plan-do-review. More specifically, the children are allowed to plan the activity that they want to complete, they completed the activity as they desired, and then they reviewed the activity with an adult. Children also were participants in large and small group activities throughout the day and had time for outdoor play. The goal for the teachers was to facilitate intellectual, social, and physical key experiences in each child’s development.

            In each of the three models, children were taught in 2 ½ hour sessions daily Monday through Friday in classrooms with a teacher to student ratio of approximately 1:8. The teachers also facilitated home-visits lasting 1 ½ hours every two weeks to answer parents questions and to aide them in demonstrating curriculum items that could be fostered in the home environment. To look at video clips that show classroom instruction click here.

            This study was longitudinal and followed the students for approximately 20 years. The children were tested at the age of ten, then again at fifteen, and for the final time at 23 years of age. The findings at age ten were that the preschool models, when implemented correctly, had the same effects on children’s intellectual and academic performance. However, this changed when the students were tested at the age of fifteen. At this testing period, the researchers included community behavior into the study and found that the students who received the Direct Instruction model reported committing 2 ½ times more acts of community misconduct than the children in the High/Scope curriculum model group. At age 23, the High/Scope group had advantages over the Direct Instruction group in the areas of years planned of education, percentage of participants living with spouse, and fewer felony arrests.

            Although this study focuses on the social effects of this curriculum model, the participants also had long-term achievement in the academic setting. These students planned to continue to receive an education despite the poverty in which they still live.

 

  • Children ages 0-5 who are of low socioeconomic status may participate in the program.
  • Curriculum engages children as active learners through hands-on experiences.
  • Teacher's role is to faciliate intellectual, social, and physical experiences to provide a strong learning environment.
  • Positve affects of the program include higher education. 

 

 

The Chicago Child-Parent Center

(Ou & Reynolds, 2006)(Grau, Clements, Reynolds, & Niles, 2004)

 

            The Chicago Child-Parent Center is a large- scale publicly funded program that began in Chicago in 1985. This project is on-going; however the following information is based on the original study conducted. 989 children, ages three and four, were selected based on a set of three conditions: the child must reside in a neighborhood that qualifies for Title I funds, the child cannot be enrolled in another preschool program, and the parents must agree to participate in the program for a set number of hours each week.

            The program is designed to focus on the development of school readiness and language skills. Parent involvement is viewed as an extremely important aspect of this program and to elicit parent volunteers, each center is equipped with a space specifically for parents to learn about the curriculum and how to help their child. The Child-Parent Centers also offer comprehensive services like free meal services and health screenings. The comprehensive services also include professional development for the teachers and instructional supplies, along with the limiting of class size to enhance school readiness. Through these domains, the program can ensure that each family has an opportunity for success in school and in later achievement.

            There are differences between this program and Head Start. Children eligible for the Head Start program is based on family-level poverty versus the neighborhood poverty criteria set forth by the Child-Parent Centers. Head Start programs are not part of the school system unlike the Child-Parent Centers that are often located within walking distance, if not inside, of the elementary school. Also, Head Start programs are only a preschool program and service only children ages three to four. The Child-Parent Center service children for up to six years, from ages three to nine. They believe this long range of services equips the student to have a better transition and success rate in school. The teachers in this program also had to meet the minimum requirement of attaining at least a Bachelor’s degree with certification in early childhood, whereas certification and levels of education required by other programs differs significantly.

            The center runs for nine months, similar to the regular school calendar and also operates a summer program for approximately eight weeks. The preschool program is a structured half-day program. Surprisingly, the Child-Parent Centers claim to use no official or uniform curriculum, but rather base their curriculum on the individual needs of the students they serve. This may be a contributing factor to their success as there are 23 centers located throughout Chicago, all servicing a vast spectrum of high poverty students. Despite this, they do report that their unified philosophy is of, “literacy and a common core of activities that include individualized instruction, small group activities, and field trips” (Ou & Reynolds, 2006, p. 181). The program emphasizes the basic skills and knowledge in literacy and mathematics and embeds these in diverse learning environments such as large group, small group, centers, individual work, and field trips.

            The findings reveal that the students who attended the Child-Parent Center had a higher level of educational attainment than the comparison group although this level was still lower than the national norm. The participants in this study also had higher levels of high school completion, highest grade completed and college attendance. These are significant findings for the continuing of preschool programs with this study serving as a model.

               In conjunction with this study, Graue and colleagues (2004) studied the curriculum used in the Child-Parent centers. Despite their evidence of having no-set curriculum, the researchers were able to discuss with the Head teachers of the program implementation to receive more background knowledge and thoughtful insights. Through these interviews, the researchers concluded that curriculum focused on high levels of teacher directed instruction was, “significantly associated with greater school readiness, word analysis and math achievement in kindergarten, 3rd and 8th reading achievement scores, and lower rates of grade retention” (Graue et al, 2004, p. 17). However, instructional methods that employed high levels of child initiated activities was associated with, “significantly greater school readiness, 3rd grade reading achievement, lower rates of grade retention, and higher rates of high school completion” (Graue et al, 2004, p. 18). This illustrates how important it is for a preschool curriculum to be a blend of both of these types. A successful program for preschoolers will be one that integrates teacher-directed lessons with child-initiated learning activities.

 

  • Children must live in a Title 1 funded neighborhood and be between the ages 0-5 years old to qualify for the program. 
  • Program promotes basic skills and knowledge in literacy and mathematics through small group, individual work, centers, and field trips. 
  • Parents or guardians are required to participate. 
  • Positive affects for participants include higher levels of high school completion and higher education attainment than those in the comparison group.

 

 

Head Start

Head Start began in 1965 as a way to fight poverty and create a "Great Society" in the United States. To read about the history of Head Start click here. As an intervention program, Head Start has evolved and has played a pivotal role in the lives of children with regard to their school preparedness, health, and social development. It is a program dedicated to the well being of young children. Head Start is not one program, but a series of locally operated programs with many incarnations. They are responsible for meeting government guidelines and belong to the National Head Start Association.

 

 

The following video showcases the need for a nurturing and educational environment as a father wishes for more for his son.

 

YouTube plugin error  

 

Head Start utilizes the three main areas of concern from the panel; literacy-focused curricula, professional development, and parental involvement. A recent Head Start Impact Study was conducted and the results were similar to those of the NELP results. Head Start has intrinsic value and former students do well in school compared to their peers. They are less likely to need special services and more likely to graduate from high school; however, many of the literacy gains that students make are in kindergarten are unremarkable at the end of first grade. More research has been called for to determine what changes can be made to assist Head Start programs in making improvements. To see the Head Start Impact Study research and press releases click here.

 

In the following video a parent is very proud of her child’s accomplishments in Head Start:

YouTube plugin error  

 

 

 

Commonalities:

 

Throughout each of these long-term studies and through the findings in the NELP report it is illustrated that programs for students, prior to kindergarten, are extremely effective and are predictors of later success in reading. There are some commonalities that these studies share that can help us to make more informed choices in regards to the curriculum and structure of these early childhood programs. Those include:

  • Classrooms that are conducive to play activities and are print-rich
  • Large/whole group meaningful activities
  • Small group/individual meaningful activities
  • Focus on early literacy strategies (phonological awareness, alphabet knowledge, print awareness, etc.)
  • Instruction is age-appropriate 
  • Oral language development
  • Parent involvement is mandatory and are well-informed
  • Mix of teacher initiated and child initiated activities during the day
  • Professional development  
  • Smaller/reduced class sizes

 

 

Practical Applications:

 

Pauline Zeece (2008) gives some wonderful thoughts and ideas about the types of books that are available to be used with children that can focus of the early learning skills that are similar to those taught in the successful projects and studies as listed above. Throughout her article, Zeece has the educator or parent question the motives of utilizing the book as well as thoughtful implications regarding book safety and young children. The list below reflects the books evaluated by Zeece in her article. To read the full article, click on the link below.

 

Click here to access the article by Pauline Zeece.

 

 

 

Ages 2-6

 

Barry, F. (2008). Big Yellow Sunflower. Somerville, MA: Candlewick Press.

Bradman, T. (2008). Daddy’s Lullaby. New York: Simon & Schuster Children’s Publishing.

Hora´cˇek, P. (2008). Look Out, Suzy Goose. Somerville, MA: Candlewick Press.

MacDonald, S. (2008). Alphabet Animals: A Slide-and-Peek Adventure. New York: Little Simon.

Wood, D. (2008). What Grandmas Can’t Do. New York: Aladdin.

 

 

Ages 4-8

 

Becker, C. (2008). A Visitor for Bear. Somerville, MA: Candlewick Press.

Dubow, J. (2007). Margaret. Glendale, CA: Action Publishing.

Frost, H. (2008). Monarch and Milkweed. New York: Atheneum.

Shahan, S. (2008). Spicy Hot Colors: Colores Picantes. Atlanta, GA: August House.

Taylor, E. (2008). My Friend the Monster. New York: Bloomsbury Children.

Thomas, J. (2008). A Birthday for Cow! New York: Harcourt Children’s Books.

Van Fleet, M. (2008). Alphabet. New York: Simon & Schuster/Paula Wiseman Books.

Willems, M. (2008). The Pigeon Wants a Puppy! New York: Hyperion Books for Children.

 

 

There are various books available that are appropriate for preschool and prekindergarten students; however, these books must be evaluated for their appropriateness to the individual child. The programs and studies like the Abecedarian Project and the Chicago Parent Child Centers pride themselves of child-appropriateness of tasks and that all materials given to the child are age-appropriate. In the article by Dwyer and Neuman (2008), a breakdown of ages and how to select books developmentally appropriate for that age range are given. These guidelines are very helpful when selecting texts to use with preschool and prekindergarten age students. To read this article, click on the link below. Click here to access the article by Dwyer and Neuman.

 

Additional Resources for Parent Child Involvement:

The New York Times Parent's Guide to the Best Books for Children: 3rd Edition Revised and Updated 

 

 

101 Activities to Help Preschoolers Excel in Reading, Writing, and Speaking

 

 

Product Details               

 

 

 

Click on the books to be directed to amazon.com for more information!

 

 

 

 

 

 

Technology Applications:

 

 

Technology is a important component of learning facilitation. The following are four kid/user friendly websites that can be used to extend literacy lessons in a classroom/home setting. 

 

 

 

www.pbskids.org/games

This site is great for literacy engagement and extensions. The site can be easily navigated students and adults.

 

 

 

http://www.starfall.com/

The starfall site is a great way to address literacy issues. This site is both free, and of high quality.  This site is easy for a child to navigate, the graphics are appealing, and children can have fun while learning. Another great feature of this site is the way it is various categories for different reading levels, styles, and interests. This site addresses different styles of literacy interests, this way students are given a variety of ways to enjoy literacy. 

The reading levels are one through four.  Level one gets students ready to read. Students can click on letters a through z and each letter has sounds and pictures to correspond.  Level two is for students who are beginning to read. These stories are easy reads that help with sight word recognition, and other reading skills. Level three is specialized with literacy through poetry, tongue twisters, music, magic, and more. Level four is full of plays, fiction and nonfiction, comics, folk tales, Greek myths, and Chinese fables.

 

 

http://www.seussville.com/

This website is great for accessing everything Dr. Seuss. The site is interactive and contains academic games and printable games. This is a good website for literacy. The graphics are colorful and simple enough for young children to understand.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

www.funbrain.com

This is an excellent website, because all of the information isn’t jumbled together. It is easy and quick to find what you are looking for. The games and interactive features would get the attention of the students. The site has pictures that correspond with each link so it is user friendly for young children. This is a great sight, because young children would be able to develop literacy and navigate the sight on their own.

 

 

https://www.virtualpre-k.org/#

This website is a virtual Pre-K for kids and parents to teach skills through videos and activities that are supported in the classroom. This site is supported by the Chicago Parent- Child centers and it also offers students and parents valuable resources to support their young readers. 

 

 

 

 

Bibliography:

 

Campbell, F. A., and Ramey, C.T. (1994). Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up      

          study of children from low-income families. Child Development, 65(2), 684-698.

Campbell, F. A., and Ramey, C.T. (1995). Cognitive and School Outcomes for High-Risk African-American Students at Middle      

          Adolescence: Positive Effects of Early Intervention. American Educational Research Journal, 32, (4 ), 743-772.

Dwyer, J. & Neuman, S.B. (2008). Selecting books for children birth through four: A developmental approach. Early Childhood

          Education Journal, 35(6), 489-494.

Grau, E., Clements, M.A., Reynolds, A.J., and Niles, M.D. (2004). More than teacher directed or child initiated: Preschool      

          curriculum type, parent involvement, and children’s outcomes in the child-parent centers. Education Policy Analysis Archives,

          12(72), 1-38.

Ou, Suh-Ruu and Reynolds, A. J. (2006). Early childhood intervention and educational attainment: age 22 findings from the

           Chicago longitudinal study. Journal of Education for Students Placed At Risk, (11)2, 175-198.

National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy: Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Washington, DC:

            National Institute for Literacy.

Schweinhart, L.J. and Weikart, D.P. (1997). The High/Scope preschool curriculum comparison study through age 23. Early      

          Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 117-143.

Zeece, Pauline (2008).  When is a Frog a Book or a Book a Frog?  Early Childhood Education Journal, 36 (3), 275-279.

 

 

 

 

Comments (0)

You don't have permission to comment on this page.